Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.

La creciente inclusión de variables socio-demográficas en los estudios de desarrollo cognitivo de niños apunta a un renovado interés de la comunidad científica por identificar cómo el contexto moldea el desarrollo humano. En esta línea de conocimiento, el presente trabajo busca contribuir al estudio de las relaciones entre desarrollo de memoria de trabajo verbal, edad, nivel socioeconómico, tipo de colegio y actividades cotidianas que realizan 159 niños colombianos de años de edad. Su memoria de trabajo verbal se evaluó empleando las pruebas de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión y Letras y Números de la escala WISC-IV. Los resultados muestran que los niños mayores se desempeñan mejor en ambas pruebas, y que los niños de estratos medios y alt... Ver más

Guardado en:

0123-9155

1909-9711

15

2011-01-01

99

109

http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2

info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess

Natalia Cadavid Ruiz, Pablo del Río - 2012

id 6e2154770e9c7243af742f3a0c2350e7
record_format ojs
spelling Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
Gelman, R.y Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigenesis. In William Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 575-630). Hoboken: John Wiley y Sons Inc.
Lyon, G. R. (1996). The Need for Conceptual and Theoretical Clarity in the Study of Attention, Memory, and Executive Function. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function. (pp. 3-9). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Law, N., Bouffet, E., Laughlin, S., Laperriere, N., Briere, M. E., Strother, D. et al., (2011). Cerebello-thalamo-cerebral connections in pediatric brain tumor patients: Impact on working memory. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2238-2248.
Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L. y Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A crosssectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 331-354.
Korkman, M. (2001). Introduction to the Special Issue on Normal Neuropsychological Development in the School-Age Years. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 325-330.
Josiane-Engel, P. M., Santos, F. H., y Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are Working Memory Measures Free of Socioeconomic Influence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, 1580-1587.
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233-253.
Gottlieb, G. (2002). From gene to organism: The developing individual as an emergent, interactional, hierarchical system. In Mark H. Johnson, Yuko Munakata y Rick O. Gilmore (Eds.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (2 ed., pp. 36-49). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., y Adamas, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(3), 265-281.
Nash, H.y Heath, J. (2011). The role of vocabulary, working memory and inference making ability in reading comprehension in down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1782-1791.
Del Río, P.y Alvarez, A. (1992). Content analysis of the television watched by spanish children. Unpublished report. Televisión Española.
Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística (s.f.) Preguntas frecuentes Estratificación Socioeconómica. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Preguntas_frecuentes_estratificación.pdf
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F. y González de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental psychology, 37(5), 715-730.
Carey, S., Gelman, R.y Jean Piaget Society (1991). The Epigenesis of mind: essays on biology and cognition. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J.y Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31-42.
Bull, R. y Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, task switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
Brown, A. B., Biederman, J., Valera, E., Makris, N., Doyle, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2011). Relationship of DAT1 and adult ADHD to task-positive and task-negative working memory networks. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193(1), 7-16.
Brocki, K. C. y Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental neuropsychology, 26(2), 571-593.
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53(2), 185-204.
Ostrosky-Solís, F., y Lozano, A. (2006). Digit Span: Effect of Education and Culture. International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 333-341.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., y Papagano, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158-173.
info:eu-repo/semantics/article
Text
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
http://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ART
http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
Wechsler, D. (2007). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia para niños IV ? Versión estandarizada. México, D.F.: Manual Moderno.
Patton, N., McDonald, C., Thomas-Tate, S.y Love, M (2010). Examining relationships among dialect variation, literacy skills and school context in first grade. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 126-145.
Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV. Adaptación española. Madrid: TEA.
Vygotski, L. S. (1987). Pensamiento y lenguaje: Teoría del desarrollo cultural de las funciones psíquicas. Buenos Aires: La Pleyade.
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D?Onofrio, B.y Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623-628.
Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled reader?s working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 1-31.
Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C.y Hasselhorn, M. (2011). Functional deficits in phonological working memory in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1934-1940.
Reynolds, C. R., Willson, V. L., y Ramsey, M. (1999). Intellectual differences among mexican americans, papagos and whites, indepent of g. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1181- 1187), 1181.
Phillips, B. M., y Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the Home Literacy: Environment of Preschool Children: A cluster analytic approach. scientific Studies of Reading, 13(2), 146-174.
Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O.y Roberts, R. J. Jr. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327-348). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Barkley, R. A. (1998). A theory of ADHD: Inhibition, executive functions, self-control, and time. In G. Reid Lyon y N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 225-260). New York: Guilford Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493.
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working Memory. Science, 31, 556- 559.
Memória de trabalho verbal
Artículo de revista
1
15
Crianças
Atividades cotidianas
Nível socioeconômico
Niños
Universidad Católica de Colombia
Actividades cotidianas
Nivel socio-económico
Memoria de trabajo verbal
del Río, Pablo
Cadavid Ruiz, Natalia
La creciente inclusión de variables socio-demográficas en los estudios de desarrollo cognitivo de niños apunta a un renovado interés de la comunidad científica por identificar cómo el contexto moldea el desarrollo humano. En esta línea de conocimiento, el presente trabajo busca contribuir al estudio de las relaciones entre desarrollo de memoria de trabajo verbal, edad, nivel socioeconómico, tipo de colegio y actividades cotidianas que realizan 159 niños colombianos de años de edad. Su memoria de trabajo verbal se evaluó empleando las pruebas de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión y Letras y Números de la escala WISC-IV. Los resultados muestran que los niños mayores se desempeñan mejor en ambas pruebas, y que los niños de estratos medios y altos que asisten a colegios privados son quienes obtienen mejores puntajes en la prueba de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión. Además, se encuentra que actividades como usar el computador, practicar un deporte y realizar los deberes escolares posibilitan una mejor ejecución en la tarea anteriormente mencionada. Los resultados de este trabajo sugieren que la tarea de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión depende de variables socio-demográficas, en tanto, la prueba de Letras y Números parece estar libre de ellas.
application/pdf
Publication
Acta Colombiana de Psicología
Anderson, V. A. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Pediatric rehabilitation, 4(3), 119-136.
Akhutina, T. V. (1997). The remediation of executive functions in children with cognitive disorders: the Vygotski-Luria neuropsychological approach. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 41(2), 144-151.
Alzate, M. C. (2006). La estratificación socioeconómica para el cobro de los servicios públicos domiciliarios en Colombia ¿Solidaridad o focalización? Santiago de Chile: Publicación de las Naciones Unidas. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Estratificacion_Solidaridad_CEPAL.pdf.
Natalia Cadavid Ruiz, Pablo del Río - 2012
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
Español
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., y Guajardo, S. (2005). The Influence of the Parents? Educational Level on the Development of Executive Functions. Developmental neuropsychology, 28(1), 539-560.
Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Akhutina, T. V. (2003). L.S. Vygotski and A.R. Luria: Foundations of Neuropsychology. Journal of Russian y East European Psychology, 41(3), 159-190.
Children
Journal article
Verbal working memory and its relationship with socio-demographic variables in colombian childrens.
Verbal working memory
Socioeconomic status
The increasing inclusion of socio-demographic variables in studies of cognitive development in children points to a renewed interest in the scientific community to identify how context shapes human development. Along this line of knowledge, this study aims to examine the relationship between development of verbal working memory, age, socioeconomic status, type of school and daily activities performed by 159 Colombian children aged 6 and 8 years old. Their verbal working memory was assessed using the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test, and the Letters and Numbers Tests of the WISC-IV. Results show that older children perform better in both tests, and that children of middle and upper classes who attend private schools are those who score higher on the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test. In addition, it was found that activities like using the computer, practicing sports and doing homework allow a better performance in the above mentioned task. Results of this study suggest that the Progression and Regression Digit Span task depends on socio-demographic variables, while the Letters and Numbers Test does not seem to be linked to them.
Daily activities
109
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/200/241
0123-9155
99
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
1909-9711
2012-01-01T00:00:00Z
2011-01-01
2012-01-01T00:00:00Z
institution UNIVERSIDAD CATÓLICA DE COLOMBIA
thumbnail https://nuevo.metarevistas.org/UNIVERSIDADCATOLICADECOLOMBIA/logo.png
country_str Colombia
collection Acta Colombiana de Psicología
title Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
spellingShingle Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
del Río, Pablo
Cadavid Ruiz, Natalia
Memória de trabalho verbal
Crianças
Atividades cotidianas
Nível socioeconômico
Niños
Actividades cotidianas
Nivel socio-económico
Memoria de trabajo verbal
Children
Verbal working memory
Socioeconomic status
Daily activities
title_short Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
title_full Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
title_fullStr Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
title_full_unstemmed Memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
title_sort memoria de trabajo verbal y su relación con variables socio-demográficas en niños colombianos.
title_eng Verbal working memory and its relationship with socio-demographic variables in colombian childrens.
description La creciente inclusión de variables socio-demográficas en los estudios de desarrollo cognitivo de niños apunta a un renovado interés de la comunidad científica por identificar cómo el contexto moldea el desarrollo humano. En esta línea de conocimiento, el presente trabajo busca contribuir al estudio de las relaciones entre desarrollo de memoria de trabajo verbal, edad, nivel socioeconómico, tipo de colegio y actividades cotidianas que realizan 159 niños colombianos de años de edad. Su memoria de trabajo verbal se evaluó empleando las pruebas de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión y Letras y Números de la escala WISC-IV. Los resultados muestran que los niños mayores se desempeñan mejor en ambas pruebas, y que los niños de estratos medios y altos que asisten a colegios privados son quienes obtienen mejores puntajes en la prueba de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión. Además, se encuentra que actividades como usar el computador, practicar un deporte y realizar los deberes escolares posibilitan una mejor ejecución en la tarea anteriormente mencionada. Los resultados de este trabajo sugieren que la tarea de Dígitos en Progresión y Regresión depende de variables socio-demográficas, en tanto, la prueba de Letras y Números parece estar libre de ellas.
description_eng The increasing inclusion of socio-demographic variables in studies of cognitive development in children points to a renewed interest in the scientific community to identify how context shapes human development. Along this line of knowledge, this study aims to examine the relationship between development of verbal working memory, age, socioeconomic status, type of school and daily activities performed by 159 Colombian children aged 6 and 8 years old. Their verbal working memory was assessed using the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test, and the Letters and Numbers Tests of the WISC-IV. Results show that older children perform better in both tests, and that children of middle and upper classes who attend private schools are those who score higher on the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test. In addition, it was found that activities like using the computer, practicing sports and doing homework allow a better performance in the above mentioned task. Results of this study suggest that the Progression and Regression Digit Span task depends on socio-demographic variables, while the Letters and Numbers Test does not seem to be linked to them.
author del Río, Pablo
Cadavid Ruiz, Natalia
author_facet del Río, Pablo
Cadavid Ruiz, Natalia
topicspa_str_mv Memória de trabalho verbal
Crianças
Atividades cotidianas
Nível socioeconômico
Niños
Actividades cotidianas
Nivel socio-económico
Memoria de trabajo verbal
topic Memória de trabalho verbal
Crianças
Atividades cotidianas
Nível socioeconômico
Niños
Actividades cotidianas
Nivel socio-económico
Memoria de trabajo verbal
Children
Verbal working memory
Socioeconomic status
Daily activities
topic_facet Memória de trabalho verbal
Crianças
Atividades cotidianas
Nível socioeconômico
Niños
Actividades cotidianas
Nivel socio-económico
Memoria de trabajo verbal
Children
Verbal working memory
Socioeconomic status
Daily activities
citationvolume 15
citationissue 1
publisher Universidad Católica de Colombia
ispartofjournal Acta Colombiana de Psicología
source https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
language Español
format Article
rights http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
Natalia Cadavid Ruiz, Pablo del Río - 2012
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
references Gelman, R.y Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigenesis. In William Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 575-630). Hoboken: John Wiley y Sons Inc.
Lyon, G. R. (1996). The Need for Conceptual and Theoretical Clarity in the Study of Attention, Memory, and Executive Function. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function. (pp. 3-9). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Law, N., Bouffet, E., Laughlin, S., Laperriere, N., Briere, M. E., Strother, D. et al., (2011). Cerebello-thalamo-cerebral connections in pediatric brain tumor patients: Impact on working memory. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2238-2248.
Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L. y Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A crosssectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 331-354.
Korkman, M. (2001). Introduction to the Special Issue on Normal Neuropsychological Development in the School-Age Years. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 325-330.
Josiane-Engel, P. M., Santos, F. H., y Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are Working Memory Measures Free of Socioeconomic Influence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, 1580-1587.
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233-253.
Gottlieb, G. (2002). From gene to organism: The developing individual as an emergent, interactional, hierarchical system. In Mark H. Johnson, Yuko Munakata y Rick O. Gilmore (Eds.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (2 ed., pp. 36-49). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., y Adamas, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(3), 265-281.
Nash, H.y Heath, J. (2011). The role of vocabulary, working memory and inference making ability in reading comprehension in down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1782-1791.
Del Río, P.y Alvarez, A. (1992). Content analysis of the television watched by spanish children. Unpublished report. Televisión Española.
Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística (s.f.) Preguntas frecuentes Estratificación Socioeconómica. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Preguntas_frecuentes_estratificación.pdf
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F. y González de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental psychology, 37(5), 715-730.
Carey, S., Gelman, R.y Jean Piaget Society (1991). The Epigenesis of mind: essays on biology and cognition. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J.y Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31-42.
Bull, R. y Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, task switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
Brown, A. B., Biederman, J., Valera, E., Makris, N., Doyle, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2011). Relationship of DAT1 and adult ADHD to task-positive and task-negative working memory networks. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193(1), 7-16.
Brocki, K. C. y Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental neuropsychology, 26(2), 571-593.
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53(2), 185-204.
Ostrosky-Solís, F., y Lozano, A. (2006). Digit Span: Effect of Education and Culture. International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 333-341.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., y Papagano, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158-173.
Wechsler, D. (2007). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia para niños IV ? Versión estandarizada. México, D.F.: Manual Moderno.
Patton, N., McDonald, C., Thomas-Tate, S.y Love, M (2010). Examining relationships among dialect variation, literacy skills and school context in first grade. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 126-145.
Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV. Adaptación española. Madrid: TEA.
Vygotski, L. S. (1987). Pensamiento y lenguaje: Teoría del desarrollo cultural de las funciones psíquicas. Buenos Aires: La Pleyade.
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D?Onofrio, B.y Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623-628.
Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled reader?s working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 1-31.
Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C.y Hasselhorn, M. (2011). Functional deficits in phonological working memory in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1934-1940.
Reynolds, C. R., Willson, V. L., y Ramsey, M. (1999). Intellectual differences among mexican americans, papagos and whites, indepent of g. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1181- 1187), 1181.
Phillips, B. M., y Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the Home Literacy: Environment of Preschool Children: A cluster analytic approach. scientific Studies of Reading, 13(2), 146-174.
Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O.y Roberts, R. J. Jr. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327-348). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Barkley, R. A. (1998). A theory of ADHD: Inhibition, executive functions, self-control, and time. In G. Reid Lyon y N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 225-260). New York: Guilford Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493.
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working Memory. Science, 31, 556- 559.
Anderson, V. A. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Pediatric rehabilitation, 4(3), 119-136.
Akhutina, T. V. (1997). The remediation of executive functions in children with cognitive disorders: the Vygotski-Luria neuropsychological approach. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 41(2), 144-151.
Alzate, M. C. (2006). La estratificación socioeconómica para el cobro de los servicios públicos domiciliarios en Colombia ¿Solidaridad o focalización? Santiago de Chile: Publicación de las Naciones Unidas. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Estratificacion_Solidaridad_CEPAL.pdf.
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., y Guajardo, S. (2005). The Influence of the Parents? Educational Level on the Development of Executive Functions. Developmental neuropsychology, 28(1), 539-560.
Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Akhutina, T. V. (2003). L.S. Vygotski and A.R. Luria: Foundations of Neuropsychology. Journal of Russian y East European Psychology, 41(3), 159-190.
type_driver info:eu-repo/semantics/article
type_coar http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
type_version info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
type_coarversion http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
type_content Text
publishDate 2011-01-01
date_accessioned 2012-01-01T00:00:00Z
date_available 2012-01-01T00:00:00Z
url https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
url_doi https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
issn 0123-9155
eissn 1909-9711
citationstartpage 99
citationendpage 109
url2_str_mv https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/200/241
_version_ 1797158757153636352